A&O – AESTHETICS – ELEMENTS of ART and AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE

ART & ORGANISM

Elements of Art and Aesthetic Experience


In ART & ORGANISM I understand ART as an ensemble of related traits that are expressed or experienced as both processes and products of cognition.  These function in ways unlike those used in most moment-to-moment, day-to-day processes of identifying and dealing with biologically relevant needs.  (Our consideration of NEEDS begins with Maslow’s hierarchy: see these now.) In other words, ART appears to “go beyond” immediate needs by exercising skills and combinations of skills (of perception, integration, and expression).  In practice this results in “making special” (Dissanyake).  That is the stimulus “stands out” and recruits more attention than it would routinely.


 COMPLEMENTARY READ: A&O on Empirical Aesthetics


 In “going beyond” the mundane, ART has much in common with PLAY.   Both ART and PLAY were once considered “autotelic” in that they were done for their own sake and not to meet some other external need.  We are now aware that they play important roles in the development that enables the fullest expression of a human’s potential.

Ethology—the study of behavior in developmental, evolutionary, ecological, and physiological context (“DEEP ETHOLOGY”)—integrates these disciplinary perspectives at every accessible level of organization from the cellular to the social in order to cultivate a holistic sense of art that will not neglect the fine-grain details on which it is built while simultaneously allowing the emergence of unique possibilities of one’s self and of humanity.  The tools of traditional empirical science paradoxically reveal mystery that seems to go beyond the simply “undiscovered” and can evoke spiritual feelings.  There is a dynamic dialectical relationship between what we feel is known and unknown, real and ideal, that manifests in thought processes and feelings, often manifest in art.   Levels of organization may begin “simple” buy vary progressively in complexity –shared characteristics of all life, or all vertebrates, or all humans in their incredible diversity still share crucial, founding elements.  This resonates with the familiar motto in aesthetics: “Unity in Diversity”   Visit A&O page on “unity in diversity”.

     

Both the expression and perception of ART are often found to be enlightening—a part of our growth, our continuing development as individuals—but like most traits these aspects of art exist along a continuum, a spectrum of more-or-less power at different ages and according to our intellectual and emotional needs.    

 


ELEMENTS of ART

·       EXPRESSIVE (meeting the artist’s needs) and/or PROJECTIVE

·        RECEPTIVE (meeting the “recipient’s” needs)


The QUALITIES OF REPRESENTATION, whether to explore our own knowledge, to communicate our knowledge to others (to extend or test or corroborate our own senses), or to teach,  are often identified in art as a discipline. The scholar of art and organism examines first the hypothesis that these qualities are more or less congenital (in-born, genetic, an evolutionary question) or acquired (learned intentionally or not, a developmental question).  Several themes emerge 

  • UnityThe idea that art manifests “unity in variety and variety in unity” (Hutcheson)[i]  reflects the connectedness of the perceptually distinctive fragments or elements of the work; often presumed to work together because of an underlying concept.  (An extreme expressions of cooperating units that appeal to different senses is the gesamtkunstwerk,  an effort to integrate multiple senses in the cause of the artwork.) [This must still endeavor to function in the context of two of the great dynamic tensions we manifest, INDIVIDUATION versus SOCIALIZATION,  and  COMPETITION versus COOPERATION).   Great pleasure derives the detection of UNITY from disparate fragments or threads. (Could this represent a bias towards the Theory of Everything?). Neurophysiological evidence has emerged from research into brain reward systems for satisfying our appetites for information, particularly when several streams converge (see “INFOVORE”).

·        Movement: the mind attends different fragments of the piece in varying orders that affect its effectiveness as a whole, and the artist can control the pattern.

o   Perception is necessarily fragmentary and phenomena are “assembled” from a flow of perceptual attention; an artist is likely to guide the senses –leading them down a path to converge on the phenomenon being represented by means of arranging the elements that will catch and guide the eye or ear (or touch or scent, or taste). 

o   “Techniques such as scale and proportion can be used to create an effect of movement in a visual artwork. For instance, an element that is further into the background is smaller in scale and lighter in value. The same element repeated in different places within the same image can also demonstrate the passing of time or movement.[3]”)

·        Variety. Diversity of elements which may act to keep sensory processes alert but nevertheless (hopefully) maintain the underlying unity. (sensory satiation)

o  Theme and Variation (“unity in variety”): the balance between unity and diversity is crucial … relates to Creative and Mundane: too creative and it won’t be recognized (Stent, Prematurity and Uniqueness) … the “baby steps” when pointed out, contextualize it  (allow it to be accommodated with minimal change)  How are things the same and different: “compare and contrast.”    This view resonates in interesting ways with the musical form, “sonata.”

·       Harmony. The effectiveness of the support that elements provide each other.  (“harmony” in cerebral functions has been invoked as a necessary condition for higher levels of consciousness) (a harmonious convergence of multiple streams of information is intrinsically pleasureable – Biederman & Vessel 2006)

·       Balance. The symmetrical, asymmetrical, or radial patterns of presentation of the elements of the work  (bilateral symmetry as an attribute of a prospective reproductive partner is important in sexual selection)

·       ContrastAny “conflict” between elements emphasizes other qualities of representation and evokes and maintains attention

·        PROPORTION. The relative size and density of elements with respect to each other (the “Golden ratio”); can support the illusion of relative distance or importance.

o   Is the Golden Ratio a significant biologically grounded proportion? Not always: Golden? See:  http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=4oyyXC5IzEE ;http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=fmaVqkR0ZXg

·       BEAUTY. “. . . the mechanism by which we come to know beauty and “feel” the aesthetic. What we “feel” at such moments is the analogy of part and whole, object and other object, relation and relation. This is one reason that in moments of aesthetic transport we assert the universality of the beautiful: we are feeling something not inchoate but precise and seemingly beyond contradiction. This is also why we feel something similar when coming upon the beautiful in art (or music) and the beautiful in nature (or mathematics). As Schopenhauer asserted, “Aesthetic pleasure is essentially one and the same, no matter whether it is evoked by a work of art or immediately produced by the contemplation of nature and life.” Beauty is experienced as a form of knowledge because it is through the archetypal rational act ‑ that of analogy and metaphor ‑ that we come to know the beautiful.” (Edward Rothstein, 1995, from Emblems of Mind: The Inner Life of Music and Mathematics, Chapter 4, “Theme and Variations: The Pursuit of Beauty” pp. 163‑168). [much more at the A&O website on BEAUTY]

  • PATTERN The way the elements are organized with respect to each other. (Science is organized knowledge. –Herbert Spencer (1861:ch. 2) 

 

·        ANALOGY –not just “ART” : arguably the CORE – the “fuel and fire” of human thinking[1]  — “long distance” analogies reach beyond familiar boundaries and even into other modalities and domains — but does art have poetic license to reach further? … as with “theme and variation”, reaching too far may break the thread for some people, disabling connections that otherwise be illuminating.    

o   Any attribute of the mind-web of memory: a detail in any modality amidst the flood of stimuli in which we are immersed, in which we swim through—color, sound, smell—a   fragment or  juxtaposition of fragments emergent from the integration of percepts, or a thread of the output of mind into action. Resemblances, connections, processes… things seen from a perspective a hair’s breadth differently [Thoreau[2], Whitman[3]]

o   In anthropomorphism, we indicate our disposition to understand the little-known in terms of the slightly-better known. —  we reason about areas of ignorance by reference to areas of less-ignorance.  We—our experiences—become the measure of the unknown, we may even validate ourselves by projecting into that void, seeking the peace of corroboration …

 

·        INFERENCE.   Much of what we understand is “inference”([4]): “deriving logical conclusions from premises known or assumed to be true.”  Conclusion derived from “circumstantial evidence”([1][5])

To mix metaphors, “Standing on the shoulders of giants” we can “extrapolate” ([2][6]) from the more-or-less solid ground on which we stand to the adjacent unknown…

Filling in gaps is the business of “interpolation” ([7]) … Coherence often assumes the existence of intermediaries that can in principle maintain an unbroken chain of causation.

“Filling in” ([8]) is an important concept in neuroscience where In vision, the phenomenon is promionenet in vision, particularly by providing probable “information across the physiological blind spot, and across natural and artificial scotomata.”

 

Probability about the validity of beliefs derives from an appatrently inborn ability[9]

  


Desmond Morris studied the biological principles of picture making and confirmed that several points apply from Leonardo da Vinci) to Congo (his research chimpanzee):                                                                                                                                                                                                                                  

Six General Principles of Aesthetic Experience and Presentation  (Morris. 1962:158):                 

·     “Autotelic” ( “Self Rewarding”)  Activation (painting can be a rewarding activity) (does this serve the goals “to know and to be known:”  BUT adaptive functions CAN be postulated

·   Compositional Control”

·   “Calligraphic Differentiation” (advancement from scribbles to circles to representation)

·   “Thematic Variation” (vastly important in the theories of aesthetics) (See VARIETY/Theme and Variation)

·   Optimum Heterogeneity” – when is a composition completed? does it depend on individual’s mood, culture? (might include relative degrees of abstraction: ambiguity)

·   “Universal Imagery” – intrinsic elements … externalized projections of some factor of biology (such as phosphenes?) … gratification in certain movements, optically pleasing (golden mean), psychological factors.


           

Desmond Morris cites Humphrey who had investigated visual preferences of primates. (He defines “preference” as a “combination of aesthetic pleasure and curiosity (or novelty)”.

1. Curiosity (interest) trumps pleasure

2. the curiosity implicit in making art making has no obvious survival value (is “autotelic” unconnected to meeting “needs”). BUT can it be a collateral expression of clearly adaptive behavioral pattern? OR a behavioral pattern that was adaptibe in ancestry.   Although closely associated with non-conscious processes, could it be a kind of “mental model making” that is more effective for being externalized (“the Corporealization of the Psyche”) … does giving pure idea a sensuous form aid in understanding or integrating the idea?

some examples of innate dispositions that may be expressed in art:

  • preference for blue/green colours [jump to safety],
  • bright light [desire to see things],

ability to compose pictures [capacity to handle notions about complex spatial relationships monkeys showed remarkably little individual variation in their preferences]                 


 

 


Six General Principles of Aesthetic Experience and Presentation   (Morris. 1962:158):

 

·    “Self Rewarding Activation (painting can be a rewarding activity) (does this serve the goals “to know and to be known”)

·     

·    “Compositional Control”

·     

·    “Calligraphic Differentiation” (advancement from scribbles to circles to representation)

·     

·    “Thematic Variation” (vastly important in the theories of aesthetics)

·     

·    “Optimum Heterogeneity” – when is a composition completed? does it depend on individual’s mood, culture? (might include relative degrees of abstraction: ambiguity)

·     

·    “Universal Imagery” – intrinsic elements … externalized projections of some factor of biology (phosphenes) … gratification in certain movements, optically pleasing (golden mean),psychological factors.                                                                                                                                                                                                           

 


Ramachandran’s “ten laws” (“eight laws of artistic experience” in Ramachndran & Hirstein (1999)

·       Peak shift  exaggerated responses to exaggerated stimuli: supernormal, caricature, selectively bemphasized key elements of stimulus.

·       Grouping– the law of “grouping” discovered by the Gestalt psychologists around the turn of the century….after several seconds you start grouping elements together, and if successful in evoking a specific meaning, an “internalrnal “Aha!” sensation as if you have just solved a problem. In short, the grouping feels good.”[10]

·       Contrast–  “…  the extraction of features prior to grouping — which involves discarding redundant information and extracting contrast—is also ‘reinforcing’. Cells in the retina, lateral geniculate body (a relay station in the brain) and in the visual cortex respond mainly to edges (step changes in luminance) but not to homogeneous surface colours; so a line drawing or cartoon stimulates these cells as effectively as a ‘half tone’ photograph. What is frequently overlooked though is that such contrast extractions — as with grouping — may be intrinsically pleasing to the eye (hence the efficacy of line drawings). Again, though, if contrast is extracted autonomously by cells in the very earliest stages of processing, why should the process be rewarding in itself?  We suggest that the answer once again has to do with the allocation of attention. Information (in the Shannon sense) exists mainly in regions of change—e.g. edges—and it makes sense that such regions would, therefore, be more attention grabbing — more ‘interesting’ — than homogeneous areas. So it may not be coincidental that what the cells find interesting is also what the organism as a whole finds interesting and perhaps in some circumstances ‘interesting’ translates into ‘pleasing’.”

·       Isolation

·       Perceptual problem solving

·       Symmetry

·       Abhorrence of coincidences/generic viewpoint

·       Repetition, rhythm and orderliness

·       Balance

·       Metaphor

 

The Journal of Consciousness Studies 6, 1999: Art and the Brain, ed. J. Goguen.

 


The development of aesthetic responses to music and their underlying neural and psychological mechanisms. Nieminen SIstók EBrattico ETervaniemi MHuotilainen M. ( 2011 Oct;47(9):1138-46. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.008. Epub 2011 May 17. )

 Abstract.  In the field of psychology, the first studies in experimental aesthetics were conducted approximately 140 years ago. Since then, research has mainly concentrated on aesthetic responses to visual art. Both the aesthetic experience of music and, especially, its development have received rather limited attention. Moreover, until now, very little attention has been paid to the investigation of the aesthetic experience of music using neuroscientific methods. Aesthetic experiences are multidimensional and include inter alia sensory, perceptual, affective, and cognitive components. Aesthetic processes are usually experienced as pleasing and rewarding and are, thus, important and valuable experiences for many people. Because of their multidimensional nature, these processes employ several brain areas. In the present review, we examine important psychological and neural mechanisms that are believed to contribute to the development of aesthetic experiences of music. We also discuss relevant research findings. With the present review, we wish to provoke further discussion and possible future investigations as we consider the investigation of aesthetic experiences to be important both scientifically and with respect to potential clinical applications.  PMID: 21665202    DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.008


 

[i] The Age of Reason saw a rise in an interest in beauty as a philosophical subject. For example, Scottish philosopher Francis Hutcheson argued that beauty is “unity in variety and variety in unity”.[10] 


[1]

[2]

 


[1] ANALOGY.     Science 3 May 2013: Vol. 340 no. 6132 pp. 550-551 DOI: 10.1126/science.1236643     BOOKS ET AL.

PSYCHOLOGY: Thinking, Broad and Deep   Keith J. Holyoak  reviews: “Surfaces and Essences Analogy as the Fuel and Fire of Thinking” by Douglas Hofstadter and Emmanuel Sander Basic Books, New York, 2013. 592 pp. $35, C$38. ISBN 9780465018475.  The reviewer is at the Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563, USA.   E-mail: holyoak@lifesci.ucla.edu

 

What makes human thinking special? Addressing the American Psychological Association a half century ago, physicist Robert Oppenheimer made the case for the centrality of analogy: “Whether or not we talk of discovery or of invention, analogy is inevitable in human thought, because we come to new things in science with what equipment we have, which is how we have learned to think, and above all how we have learned to think about the relatedness of things” (1). In Surfaces and Essences, Douglas Hofstadter (Indiana University) and Emmanuel Sander [University of Paris (Saint-Denis)] build the case that the ability to see analogies indeed forms the core of human thinking—the way we “think about the relatedness of things.”

 

No one is better equipped to make the case for analogy than the senior author. Hofstadter, analogist extraordinaire, burst onto the stage of cognitive science in 1979 with his Pulitzer Prize–winning Gödel, Escher, Bach, in which he created playful analogies and allegorical dialogues to illuminate such mathematical abstractions as recursion and undecidability (2). His intellectual breadth—trained in mathematics and physics, professor of computer science and psychology, artist, translator of Russian poetry and French novels—brings with it the capacity to see long-distance connections between situations and ideas, abstract their essences, and ground these abstractions in illuminating analogies. Surfaces and Essences, though not a sequel to Gödel, Escher, Bach, inherits a good deal of its intellectual focus and playful spirit. Like a strong marriage, the collaboration between Hofstadter and Sander, a French psychologist, is sufficiently seamless that the book reads as a single voice. Sander deserves credit for bringing in a salutary dose of psychological research, particularly on mathematical problem-solving and education, where goals and causal understanding are critical in distinguishing essence from surface. The mathematician soars among pure patterns; the psychologist stays rooted in human concerns. Their Anglo-Francophone collaboration reifies the art of translation, one of the most complex types of analogy-making. Rather than producing a conventional translation from source to target language, the authors worked in parallel on English and French versions. The book includes an illuminating self-referential sketch of its bilingual origin.

Hofstadter and Sander’s thesis—analogy is the core of cognition—is less (or more) radical than it might sound, as they extend analogy to include “categorization through analogy-making.” One situation is compared to another—two faces, two dogs, a heart and a pump—yielding a proto-category, to be refined by additional examples. Categories are not fixed and final products but are endlessly extensible by analogy. Waves on water come to embrace sound waves, then light waves, then spin waves, and then probability waves, as the concept wave becomes increasingly abstract. The authors convincingly refute those enthusiasts of embodied cognition who assume that because concepts are typically grounded in human perception and action, abstraction has been explained away. No: “abstraction is key, and to leave it out of one’s theory of thinking is to miss the boat by a wide margin.”

   View larger version:   In this page   In a new window   Download PowerPoint Slide for Teaching   Abstracted by analogy. Waves on water are mapped to sound waves, then light waves, and then probability waves.   CREDIT: AIROM BLEICHER

The book grounds its abstractions in a garden of delightful examples: analogies based on words, phrases, metaphors, and proverbs; “me, too” stories where one person’s anecdote elicits an analogical reminding in a listener; slips of action and of the tongue. Lofty scientific analogies are foreshadowed by the “banalogies” of everyday cognition. An elderly father driving by a cemetery baffles his adult son with the remark, “This is where all four of your grandkids were born”—the intended “all four of your grandparents are buried” fell victim to analogical slippage. As a child analogizes a toy truck to a real one, so Galileo analogized from Earth’s one-of-a-kind Moon to hypothesize the moons of Jupiter (exemplifying “meta-analogy”). In the final chapter, Hofstadter the mathematician-physicist provides a compelling exposition of the analogical origins of number concepts and Einstein’s relativity theory. Over a page of the book’s index is devoted to the entry “lists” (e.g., “of abstract uses of ‘mother,’” “of sour grapes situations,” “of computer concepts used in daily life”). Extensive endnotes and references provide an excellent overview of scholarly sources.

 

The authors provide a cornucopia of analogical examples and qualitative insights but largely bypass computational and neural constraints on analogy (e.g., the critical concept of “binding” is not discussed). The influence of prior experience on cognition is indeed ubiquitous, but is it always “analogy”? People (and other animals) also learn by conditioning, passive accumulation of statistical associations, and other implicit mechanisms [e.g., (3)]. The authors appear to be of two minds on the question of whether analogy is unique to humans. Like Darwin before them (4), they are avowed dog fanciers and similarly apply the most naïve of analogies—anthropomorphism—to their canine friends: “categorization for a dog is clearly the creation of analogical bridges to prior knowledge.” But a later section titled “What Makes Homo Sapiens Sapiens Sapiens?” reads like a retraction. The uniquely human core of analogy—the ability to encode and flexibly re-represent “the relatedness of things”—accounts for the fact that to date, scientists have emerged in only one species.

Surfaces and Essences warrants a place alongside Gödel, Escher, Bach and major recent treatments of human cognition (5). Analogy is not the endpoint of understanding, but its indispensable beginning. As Oppenheimer observed, “We cannot learn that we have made a mistake unless we can make a mistake; and our mistake is almost always in the form of an analogy to some other piece of experience” (1).

 

References

1.     R. Oppenheimer, Am. Psychol. 11, 127 (1956). CrossRefWeb of ScienceSearch Google Scholar

 

2.     D. R. Hofstadter, Gödel, Escher, Bach: An Eternal Golden Braid (Basic, New York, 1979).

Search Google Scholar

3.     B. J. Knowlton, J. A. Mangels, L. R. Squire, Science 273, 1399 (1996).  Abstract

4.     D. C. Penn, K. J. Holyoak, D. J. Povinelli, Behav. Brain Sci. 31, 109, discussion 130 (2008).

MedlineSearch Google Scholar

5.     D. Kahneman, Thinking, Fast and Slow (Farrar, Straus and Giroux, New York, 2011); reviewed in (6)Search Google Scholar

6.     S. J. Sherman, Science 334, 1062 (2011).   Abstract/FREE Full Text


[2]AIt is only necessary to behold the least fact or phenomenon, however familiar, from a point a hair’s breadth aside from our habitual path or routine, to be overcome, enchanted by its beauty and significance … To perceive freshly, with fresh senses is to be inspired.@ (Thoreau, Journal Dec 11 1855 8:44)

[3] “Do I contradict myself? / Very well then I contradict myself, /(I am large, I contain multitudes.) (Whitman, Song of Myself) 

[4]

[5] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Circumstantial_evidence: “Circumstantial evidence is evidence that relies on an inference to connect it to a conclusion of fact—like a fingerprint at the scene of a crime. By contrast, direct evidence supports the truth of an assertion directly—i.e., without need for any additional evidence or inference. … On its own, it is the nature of circumstantial evidence for more than one explanation to still be possible. Inference from one piece of circumstantial evidence may not guarantee accuracy. Circumstantial evidence usually accumulates into a collection, so that the pieces then become corroborating evidence. Together, they may more strongly support one particular inference over another. An explanation involving circumstantial evidence becomes more valid as proof of a fact when the alternative explanations have been ruled out. … Circumstantial evidence is especially important in civil and criminal cases where direct evidence is lacking.”

[6]http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Extrapolation: “In mathematics, extrapolation is the process of estimating, beyond the original observation range, the value of a variable on the basis of its relationship with another variable …. Extrapolation may also apply to human experience to project, extend, or expand known experience into an area not known or previously experienced so as to arrive at a (usually conjectural) knowledge of the unknown…”

[7]http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Interpolation : “… interpolation is a method of constructing new data points within the range of a discrete set of known data points. In engineering and science, one often has a number of data points, obtained by sampling or experimentation, which represent the values of a function for a limited number of values of the independent variable. It is often required to interpolate (i.e. estimate) the value of that function for an intermediate value of the independent variable. …  A different problem which is closely related to interpolation is the approximation of a complicated function by a simple function.”

[9] NATURE News Humans have innate grasp of probabilityStudy of indigenous Maya people finds probabilistic reasoning does not depend on formal education.  Ewen Callaway  03 November 2014

People overrate the chances of dying in a plane crash and guess incorrectly at the odds that a coin toss will yield ‘heads’ after a string of several ‘tails’. Yet humans have an innate sense of chance, a study of indigenous Maya people suggests. Adults in Guatemala who have never learned a formal number system or a written language did as well as formally educated adults and children at estimating the probability of chance events1, the researchers found.

Children are born with a sense of number, and the roots of our mathematical abilities seem to exist in monkeys, chickens and even salamanders. But evidence has suggested that the ability to assess the chances of a future event is not as innate.

In a 1972 study, Daniel Kahneman, a psychologist at Princeton University in New Jersey, and the late psychologist Amos Tversky found that educated adults incorrectly judged the sequence of coin tosses ‘heads-heads-heads-tails-tails-tails’ as less probable than ‘heads-tails-heads-tails-tails-heads’2. (Any such sequence has the same exact probability, 1/64, of occurring.) Other researchers have pointed to the fact that the mathematics of probability were not worked out until the seventeenth century to argue that probabilistic reasoning is not innate and relies on formal education.

More recent research has pointed to a primitive sense of probability. In a study published in December 2013 and titled “Apes are intuitive statisticians”, researchers found that chimpanzees, gorillas and other great apes made decisions on the basis of the chances of receiving a preferred treat such as a banana over a less-coveted carrot3.

Vittorio Girotto, a cognitive scientist at the University IUAV of Venice, Italy, and his colleagues have found in past work that young children have some grasp of probability, albeit with limits4. For instance, 12-month-old babies shown three yellow balls and one blue ball being put into a container expressed surprise when a blue ball emerged. Yet 3- and 4-year-olds answer at random when asked which colour will be pulled from the container, and older children who passed that task struggled at more complicated tests of probabilistic reasoning.

Running the numbers

To further probe humans’ innate sense of probability, Girotto’s IUAV colleague Laura Fontanari travelled to rural Guatemala to work with adults from the indigenous Kaqchikel and K’iche people who had not been formally educated in language or maths. In a series of tests of probabilistic reasoning, the adults performed just as well as Maya schoolchildren and Italian adults.

Related stories:  Scientific method: Statistical errors …  Dyscalculia: Number games  … Animal InstinctsMore related stories

The tests involved picking the colour of a chip drawn at random from a pool of several. If the pool contained three blue chips and one yellow chip, for example, most of participants guessed that the chip chosen at random would be blue.

The Maya adults also updated their predictions with new information. In a test in which a pot contained four square-shaped chips (all of them red) and four circular chips (one red, three green), they determined that a red chip of any shape was most likely to be drawn. But when the researchers told them that a circular chip would be drawn, the volunteers updated their decisions and picked green.

In a third test, participants were shown a collection of differently-coloured tokens and asked to bet on whether two tokens chosen at random would be the same colour. Schoolchildren under 6 tend struggle with such combinatorial probabilities. But Mayan adults and 9-year-olds and Italian adults all performed better than chance.The results were published on 3 November in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences1.

Girotto says that people’s problems estimating probabilities may have to do with how uncertainty is expressed. Tasks that people struggle with, such as those presented by Kahneman and Tversky, often involved reading about percentages, while Girotto’s tests were visual.

“Eventually we will have a map of boundaries between the tasks that untrained people or other animals can perform and those they fail,” says Kahneman. “The present study is very useful in allowing us to colour one segment of that large map — but we should not draw overly general conclusions from it.”

Girotto also distinguishes between innate ability and advanced understanding. “The fact that we discovered this intuition in infants and in preliterate adults does not mean that this form of reasoning is flawless,” he says.

Nature  doi:10.1038/nature.2014.16271

References

1.    Fontanari, L., Gonzalez, M., Vallortigara, G. & Girotto, V. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USAhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1410583111 (2014).

Show context

2.    Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. Cognitive Psychol. 3, 430–454 (1972).

o   Article

Show context

3.    Rakoczy, H. et alCognition 131, 60–68 (2014).

o   Article

o   PubMed

o   ISI

Show context

4.    Téglás, E., Girotto, V., Gonzalez, M. & Bonatti, L. L. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104,19156–19159 (2007).

o   Article

o   PubMed

Show context

Related stories and links

From nature.com

·        Scientific method: Statistical errors  12 February 2014

·        Dyscalculia: Number games  09 January 2013

·        Animal Instincts  22 December 2010

·        Babies have an eye for statistics  31 March 2008

·        Statistics starts young  20 February 2001

[10]The different extrastriate visual areas may have evolved specifically to extract correlations in different domains (e.g. form, depth, colour), and discovering and linking multiple features (‘grouping’) into unitary clusters — objects — is facilitated and reinforced by direct connections from these areas to limbic structures. In general, when object-like entities are partially discerned at any stage in the visual hierarchy, messages are sent back to earlier stages to alert them to certain locations or features in order to look for additional evidence for the object (and these processes may be facilitated by direct limbic activation).”


last update 23 April 2018