GREENBERG (2003) sociality stress and corpus striatum

PHYSIOLOGY & BEHVIOR

(2003) 79(3):429-440

 

 

 

 

Sociality, Stress, and the Corpus Striatum

of the Green Anolis Lizard

Neil Greenberg

 

Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 37996, USA

 

Abstract:

      The green anolis lizard, Anolis carolinensis, is a uniquely convenient species with great potential for providing insights about the causes and consequences of social behavior from an evolutionary perspective.  In this species, social interactions are mediated by visual displays in which specific units of behavior are combined in various ways to communicate several  more-or-less specific messages.  Two related research programs that utilize this species converge in provocative ways to provide insight into this phenomenon.   The first program is centered on the basal ganglia, now known to be crucial to the expression of aggressive territoriality in this species, and the second research program examines the way the physiological stress response is involved in aggression and its subsequent adaptive outcomes.  Both the neural and the neuroendocrine systems affect the progress of social interactions as well as the subsequent social dominance relationships when combatants subsequently live together.  Further, because body color depends almost exclusively on the stress response, skin color provides a unique in-situ bioassay of otherwise inaccessible information about the animal’s internal state.  The fullest understanding of the physiological ethology of this model species will depend on an interdisciplinary approach that considers both proximate (physiological) and ultimate (evolutionary) causes of displays.  Questions thus arising include how the nervous system controls and assembles the specific units of behavior –motor patterns and autonomic reflexes– into displays that are adaptive in specific contexts.

 

 

1.  Introduction

      Between stimulus and action, the intervening neurobiology of display behavior is poorly understood.  Displays of more or less complexity are manifest in all taxa and are often presumed to represent or have evolved from chains of reflexes or fixed action patterns (FAPs).  The adaptive value of modulating and coordinating such behavioral patterns to help organisms deal with vagaries, exigencies, and emerging challenges of their environments is a major force in the evolution of the brain.    Among the best studied of these behavioral patterns are social displays, and among the most interesting of these displays are those of lizards.  Among lizards, the green anole, Anolis carolinensis, may be the most studied. 

      Two ideas converge in this brief account of the neuroethology of display behavior in the green anole: the role of the basal ganglia in the coordination and expression of social displays and the influence of the physiological stress response on displays during and subsequent to aggressive encounters.  First I will review the social behavior of the green anolis lizard with an emphasis on units of behavior.   Then I will review and discuss brain research on the social displays of the green anole inspired by and first done in collaboration with Paul D. MacLean [Greenberg, et al 1979], and more recently extended by Cliff Summers [for example, Summers and Greenberg 1995, Summers  et al. 1998] and Lewis Baxter [for example, Baxter 2001].  Next, research on the interplay of stress endocrinology, brain, and behavior will be outlined.  This work followed the brain research but was inspired by Daniel Lehrman and David Crews, and often researched in collaboration with Crews [e.g., Greenberg, et al 1984].  Finally, I’ll comment on implications of these projects for understanding the evolution of brain and behavior.

2.  The green anole – a model reptile. 

      The small, diurnal, arboreal lizard, the green anole (Anolis carolinensis) is one of the most scrutinized lizards in science, and a valuable model for several biomedical research programs [Greenberg, et al 1989]. Specific elements of social behavior have been studied and reported since the 1930s [Evans 1936, Greenberg and Noble 1944, and Noble and Bradley 1933].   More recent detailed ethological accounts were prepared in support of neuroethological studies of social behavior [Greenberg 1977b]. These and subsequent reports detailing the display behavior of the species [Jenssen, et al 2000] and its behavioral ecology [Jenssen 1978, Jenssen, et al 1995] as well as behavioral endocrinology of reproductive patterns [Crews 1979, Crews 1980] have contributed to a detailed inventory of behavioral patterns characteristic of the species (Table 1).             

 

TABLE 1. UNITS of BEHAVIOR

 

2.1.  Units of behavior.   

      Many social displays are found to consist of multiple units of behavior, the forms and coordination of which are valuable sources of clues about the evolutionary background to the behavioral pattern.  An inventory of 50 units of behavior (“ethogram”) associated with sociality has been developed for the green anole (Table 1).  It is important to note that units of behavior in such lists must be identified with as little reference as possible to function  because it is a common observation in comparative behavior studies that similar behavioral patterns can serve very different functions in different individuals or species (or in the same individual at different times).  The units in Table 1 are also identified with respect to the life-history contexts in which they appear – maintenance behavior (such as foraging, feeding, defecating, grooming), aggression (territorial defense and conspecific fighting); reproductive behavior (courtship, mating, egg-laying); and nonspecific defensive behavior.  The occurrence of specific units of behavior in either multiple categories or in very restricted contexts suggests more-or-less conservatism in their stimulus control as well as hypotheses about their proximate (physiological) causes and consequences.   For example, some units of display behavior are commonly seen expressed in the absence of any specific stimulus (dewlap) and suggest nothing more than elevated arousal while others (such as rapid nodding) are restricted to very specific contexts that may require endocrine priming and a specific stimulus (receptive female).

      Most units of behavior fit the criterion of reflexes or “fixed action pattern.”  A reflex is often regarded as the simplest of behavioral units. They are highly stereotyped and can be chained together in cascades that of highly complex motor patterns.  A fixed-action pattern (FAP), on the other hand, refers to a more complex ensemble of motor acts orchestrated into a performance involving an unlearned stereotyped temporal and spatial pattern.  The term, “fixed action pattern” is a misleading translation of the original German, Erbkoordination, which is more correctly rendered as “inherited movement coordination” [Heymer 1977].  When found to be species-typical, FAPs are regarded much like a morphological trait as a distinctive attribute of a particular species.

2.2.  The social behavior of the green anole. 

      What follows is an account of social behavior seen in naturalistic laboratory vivaria.  In many specific details, particularly those involving fixed action patterns and social displays, they are much like those seen in the field.  Still, there are important differences [see Jenssen, et al 1995], apparently attributable to the larger diversity of alternative actions available in the field, but also as a result of the opportunities for closer scrutiny in the laboratory.  Ideally, findings in both kinds of studies would inform each other in reciprocal fashion [Greenberg 1995, Schnierla 1950].

      In nature, male green anoles emerge from seasonal inactivity and establish territories by aggressively competing with other males of the same species [see Crews 1979 but also Jenssen, et al 2001 for key differences between laboratory and field].  Species recognition and subsequent competition usually involves exchanges of distinctive displays.  When aggressive, animals will face-off and begin circling each other, displaying occasionally, possibly jaw-sparring, (see Table 1) and manifesting signs of acute stress as their body color changes (see below); only rarely is physically dangerous combat observed – the contest appears to be one of stamina.  Losers typically flee, but there is some evidence that in nature they may remain in a winners territory as a social subordinate.   A typical first display has been termed  “assertion,” and includes a distinctive pattern of vertical movements of the head performed with more-or-less amplitude (head-nods) that provides the definitive species-typical “signature” display for many lizards [Heymer 1977], the green anole included.   The assertion display consists of such head-nods emphasized with coordinated push-ups and accompanied by a brief dewlap extension (Tables 1, 2)       

      In the lab as in the field, males often “spontaneously” manifest assertion displays often as they move about “patrol” their territories.  At least there is no external stimulus the human observer can detect that might evoke such displays.  Such displays suggest elevated non-specific arousal rather than a response to any specific evocative stimulus. The display also serves as an “advertisement.”  If a male’s assertion display is observed by another male not previously observed, that second male may call attention to himself by reacting with his own sequence of headnods coordinated with push-ups and extension of the dewlap and the resident, observing this, may then rapidly escalate its display into “threat” (with extended throat only) or “challenge.”  In this display, the elements of assertion are complemented by extended throat and sagittal expansion (of the body profile) and (in interactions of sufficient duration) erection of nuchal and dorsal crests along the back, all effectively enlarging the animal’s apparent size.   If a male’s display is observed by a female, on the other hand, her head-nod response will cause the aggressive male to switch to “courtship:” he will approach the female with a unique “strutting” gait punctuated by one or more series of rapid nods.   Interestingly, this head-nod display (no push-ups or dewlap) is also occasionally performed by males defeated in combat and are taken by some to express “subordination” (Table 2).

      Displays exchanged between two lizards are easily studied in the laboratory by carefully removing an opaque divider between two vivaria in which the animals appear acclimated.  When two males, each the exclusive occupant of adjacent vivarium, have their divider removed, they each act as though the other is an intruder in their territory [details in Greenberg 1992].  This procedure minimizes the stress of handling or an observer effect.  Reproductively competent males that see each other thus, almost always respond to each other with an assertion or challenge display.  When territorial males escalate their competition, a full “challenge” display is seen.  This is an assertion display (the species-typical component) complemented by postural changes (“modifiers”).  An early response might also be extended throat” but an experienced aggressor might rapidly effect the challenge display, in which an enlarged sagittal profile of his body complements the assertion display to the intruder. As aggressive encounters proceed, the male’s behavior is accompanied by autonomic responses: Body color may at first darken rapidly and then revert back to green –but with a critical difference, a dark eyespot will appear just rostral to the eye.  In some cases, the initial darkening does not occur and the animal’s color changes quickly to green.  In as little as 30 seconds, a crest of erectile tissue will appear along its neck and back.  The antagonist typically responds in kind and they stalk each other with slow, deliberate, apparently tense movements; the tips of their tails may twitch.  Prolonged encounters by evenly matched males may result in “jaw-sparring” or (more rarely) “jaw-locking” and biting (Table 1).   Most commonly however, fights conclude with no trauma to either combatant.

      In the course of such extended interactions, the animals appear to assess their position relative to each other. This may be reflected in multiple changes between green and brown body color, although once present, the eyespot will remain.   There is evidence that the eyespot can serve as a signal that evokes sympathetic activation and inhibits aggression in conspecifics [Korzan, et al 2002].   Body color “reversals” of aggressively engaged lizards is attributable to highly elevated epinephrine and sometimes they go directly from green to green with an eyespot and apparently skip the intervening brown phase.  Colors may then darken considerably and in (rare) extreme situations color may become blotchy (Table 1).  As aggressive posturing and displaying subsides, the animal that is brown is probably the one subdued, typically lowering their chins to the substrate (head down).  Even if clinging to the underside of a limb, the “loser” will press his chin to the perch surface.  The winner climbs to the top of his perch and may perform a few assertion displays with his head raised (“head-up-high”).  The head-lowering of losers and raising of winners (seen in many reptile taxa) may be a potential evolutionary origin of the bobbing display, corresponding to Desmond Morris’s category of “alternating ambivalent movements” in his analysis of ritualization [Morris 1956].

      After territorial confrontations, winners, apparently little affected, return to their routine, while losers, if forced by the vivarium or environmental circumstances to remain in sight of the winner, change in obvious ways:  They behave as social subordinates, selecting lower perches and not court females; they also manifest a brown body color most of the time [Greenberg, et al 1984]. There may be brief aggressive skirmishes for another day or two, but by day 3, the relationship seems stabilized and such pairs can, in the laboratory, cohabit for extended periods.  The preference for lower perches and the disinterest in females appears to be more an altered motivational state rather than a response learned in the presence of the winning male – even when the dominant is removed, subordinates may take up to two days to recover the former habits.     

      The several types of social displays identified in the green anole represent more or less specificity in form and of stimulus control.  There is an apparently highly conservative central species-typical element (the head nod) the meaning of which is modified by the coordinated expression of hormone-dependent or context-dependent display components (Table 2).  The display modifiers have been characterized as “static” (such as crests or eyespot) or “dynamic” (such as a pulse of dewlap erection) by Jenssen [Jenssen 1978].

 

TABLE 2

Shared Elements of Social Displays in the Green Anole

                                             Display Components                                                                          

Display Context

Head-nod

push-up

Dewlap

Extended Throat

Sagittal Expansion

Rapid  Nodding

“Subordination”

T

 

 

 

 

 

“Assertion”

T

T

T

 

 

 

“Threat”

T

T

T

T

 

 

“Challenge”

T

T

T

T

T

 

“Courtship”

T

T

T

 

 

T

                                                                                   

 

2.3. Assembling the units of behavior

      Taken together, the information in Tables 1 and 2 indicate that the species-typical bobbing pattern, while stereotyped, is evoked by a broad spectrum of situations but specific units of behavior are added or deleted in more limited contexts, presumably to modify the meaning of the display [Jenssen 1978, Jenssen 1979].   Head nods are commonly emphasized with forelimb movements and a brief dewlap extension, which as an ensemble constitute the assertion (“signature”) display.  Complementing the bobbing pattern and observed in more specific contexts are slight variations such as rapid nods (“courtship”), a hindlimb contribution to the head-nodding movement (four-leg pushup), erection of a fleshy nuchal crest (“challenge”), or erection of parts of the hyoid apparatus (“assertion” or “threat”).  The hyoid can be erected in two stages: extension of the long retrobasal process will extend the dramatic red dewlap (an element of the “assertion” display) and extension of the basihyal element (the fulcrum upon which the retrobasal element rests) will simply enlarge the apparent size of the throat (extended throat, “threat”). The expression of these and related units of behavior are presumed to have become progressively more precise and stereotyped because of an advantage that precision confers, such as the correct identification of the species or gender doing the display or the motivational state of the performer [but see Hauser 1996].  Autonomic responses include color changes (of which only the eyespot appears to have signal value – see below).

 

3.  Basal ganglia influences social displays of anolis

Social displays are of great intrinsic interest, but their relative stereotypy and well understood stimulus control provides powerful models for structuring investigations of neural mechanisms.  The basal ganglia are of particular interest because of their long historical association with control of motor sequences.  “Basal ganglia” is an alternate term for an array of related structures called the striatal complex, nicknamed the R-complex by Paul D. MacLean (“R” for “reptilian”) because of its remarkable prominence in reptiles, seemingly corresponding to evolutionary innovations in behavior first seen in reptiles [see MacLean 1990].   The basal ganglia include the corpus striatum (caudate and putamen) and is sometimes termed “non-limbic” or dorsal” striatum.  The putamen is so intermeshed with an afferent projection (the globus pallidus, pallidum) that the two structures are occasionally regarded together as the lenticular nucleus.  The nucleus accumbens is, along with the olfactory tubercle, sometimes called “ventral” or “limbic” striatum.   Caudate, putamen, and globus pallidus are sometimes referred to collectively as “neostriatum” while nucleus accumbens, olfactory tubercle and ventral pallidum are called “paleostriatum” (PS) [Greenberg 2002a].  Closely associated structures are the substantia nigra (possessing reciprocal connections with the dorsal striatum) and the ventral tegmental area (possessing reciprocal connections with the ventral striatum) [Parent 1986].

      The reptilian paleostriatum (PS) was the focus of an investigation of forebrain control of display behavior in the green anole.  Paul D. MacLean’s finding that lesions of the globus pallidus in squirrel monkeys disrupts species-typical displays [MacLean 1978] and studies that indicated that species-typical displays could be reliably evoked in a reptile [Greenberg 1977, Greenberg 1978], converged at MacLean’s Laboratory of Brain Evolution and Behavior at the National Institute of Mental Health on an investigation of the function of the PS in green anolis display behavior.  Preceding investigations of forebrain influences on reptilian behavior [Distel 1978, Sugarman 1978, Tarr 1982] used stimulation techniques and provided clues about our candidate sites for the neural control of displays but were not conclusive.  With the help of a forebrain atlas developed for A. carolinensis [Greenberg 1982], the paleostriatal complex was probed with microlesions [Hews and Worthington 2001]. 

      The lesion studies took advantage of the absence of a corpus callosum in this taxon, providing us with a natural split-brain preparation.  By making only unilateral lesions and directing visual input to the lesioned or the intact hemisphere, each individual served as its own progressive matched control [Greenberg, et al 1979]; animal care and experimental protocols in Greenberg 1992]. There was little concern about lateralized brain function, but hemispheres to be lesioned were selected at random.  Recent findings of right versus left dominance in control of lizard aggression [Hews and Worthington 2001] were not observed in these cases.

      Lesioned lizards recovered very rapidly and appeared amazingly unaffected.  Most animals remained alert, foraged and fed as normal, often expressing the assertion display.  Only when an intruding conspecific was provided was  a profound deficit observed [Greenberg 1977].  When vision was restricted to the lesioned hemisphere, the subject remained responsive to presence of an intruder but was unresponsive to the releasers of territorial aggression it provided [Greenberg, et al 1979], a behavioral deficit that might be characterized as “social agnosia.”

      More recent investigations of basal ganglia in social displays of green anoles have been conducted by Lewis Baxter, who initiated a series of experiments based on his insight [Baxter 1999] that the control of the stereotyped displays of anoles shared some features with the control of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Using the Anolis lizard model, he showed that beyond a sharp increase in forebrain serotonin during dominant displays and a decrease during subordinate displays, there was an activation of dorsolateral basal ganglia and deactivation of the ventromedial area.   Related experiments went further to analyze the subtypes of serotonin receptors and their distribution in Anolis carolinensis, confirming important commonalities with other taxa [Clark, et al 2000a].  In a series of analyses on dopamine receptors, the occurrence, distribution, and pharmacological specificity of dopamine D1 and D2 receptor sub-types were also seen to be similar to those of  mammals. One interesting difference, however, is that neural tissue in the parts of basal ganglia outside the ventral striatum characterized by D1 and D2 receptor subtypes is largely separated, rather than co-mingled as in mammalian basal ganglia [Clark, et al 2000b].

 

4.  Neurotransmitters in the behavior of anoles

  The relatively stable changes in body color that accompany reduced social status in green anoles indicate important changes in endocrine tone. Might the differences in the display behavior of social dominants and subordinates be attributable to comparable changes in the brain?  Cliff Summers led a series of investigations that analyzed specific neural structures isolated by  micropunches through slabs of brain tissue.  The use of Coulochem electrode array high pressure liquid chromatography allowed analysis of specific sites in the brains of dominant and subordinate males for indolamines, catecholamines, and their metabolites [summarized in Summers 2001, Summers 2002].   We learned that central serotonin production and turnover is more rapidly activated in losers of fights (destined to behave in a subordinate way) than in winners. A closer analysis revealed that serotonergic activity in dominants and subordinates had a distinctive time course as well as regional distribution in the brain [Summers, et al 1998].

      There is abundant evidence that changes in serotonergic activity in the brain is associated with stress and subordinate social  behavior in many taxa including Anolis [see Baxter 2001, Larson and Summers 2001].   In green anoles, the greatest serotonergic changes were detected in the telencephalon of subordinate males.  One hour after a fight, hippocampal cortex and  nucleus accumbens showed increased ratios of 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid/serotonin.  Just as in earlier studies of the brainstems of these animals [Summers and Greenberg 1995], the ratio gradually decreased as the animal’s social status became consolidated, and within one month, ratios had returned to normal.  Measured in the brains of lizards sacrificed at an hour, day, week, and month following a fight, changes were seen to be more rapid in dominant males.  The patterns of serotonergic activation are so similar in hippocampus, nucleus accumbens and brainstem that a co-ordinated response may be involved in mediating short-term social stress and aggression. Similarly, medial and lateral amygdala exhibit corresponding, but delayed patterns in subordinate males, suggest a co-ordinated response in these regions mediating a longer-term stress response. [Summers, et al 1998; summarized in Summers 2001, Summers 2002] Work on free-ranging spiny lizards (Sceloporus jarrovi) provided consilient findings: brain serotonin activity and turnover was greater in non-territorial males than in those holding territories [Matter, et al 1998].  Comparable findings in fish [Winberg, et al 1992] and mammals [Yodyingyuad, et al 1985, in primates], suggest a phylogenetically conserved mechanism of monoamine behavioral modulation.

 

5.0.  Stress in Anoles

5.1.  Body color in Anolis is uniquely sensitive to stress hormones.   

      The body color changes seen in green anoles occasionally during their maintenance and often when subjected to any of a wide array of disturbances has given rise to their popular nickname, “American chameleon.”  The appearance of a potential predator in the field or a careless observation protocol in the lab, will evoke a brown body color.  During aggressive interactions (as mentioned above) color shifts also occur as interactions proceed.  Both males will likely develop eyespots early in their interaction, but near their conclusion, probable losers are usually brown, and winners green.

      Unlike chameleons and other lizards investigated, body color changes are attributable only to circulating hormones (Figure 1). A role for direct neural control was excluded in a series of studies by Kleinholz [Kleinholz 1938a, Kleinholz 1938b].  This allows body color in green anoles to serve as a partial in situ assay of the endocrine tone of the chromo-active hormones: epinephrine (EPI), norepinephrine (NE) and melanotropin (MSH). Several patterns of body color can be distinguished (Table 1) that suggest the underlying acute flux of circulating hormones. Body color can also be affected by non-social activities such as predator avoidance, but generally in contexts reasonably construed as stressful.  A shift from green to brown, or darkening involving speckling and the appearance of a small “eyespot” just behind the eye, indicate specific patterns of activation of alpha2- and beta2-adrenoceptors (sympathetic elements of the acute stress response in Figure 1), and MSH. 

5.2.  Stress responses are both causes and consequences of social dominance relationships

      Stress is inevitably evoked in agonistic or competitive behavior.  Our research has shown that in the dominant-subordinate relationships that are established by green anoles in laboratory vivaria, the typically brown subordinates have elevated circulating corticosterone levels [Greenberg, et al 1984] and lowered androgen [Greenberg and Crews 1990].   Subordinates also manifest changes in activity of central neurotransmitters consistent with elevated stress and lowered aggression [see Summers 2002 and Summers, et al 1998 and references therein). It is significant that elevated circulating corticosterone levels can potentially result in a shift in the ratio of EPI to NE released from the adrenal chromaffin tissue because of its facilitation of a key enzyme in the conversion of NE to E.  An increase in E relative to NE is associated with behavioral patterns characteristic of fearful or subordinate animals.  Further, relative autonomic tone of the two combatants may be important: the male that first manifests the eyespot invariably wins the contest [Summers and Greenberg 1994].

 

FIGURE 1 STRESS and CHROMOACTIVE HORMONES

 

6.  Prospects for insights about the evolution of brain and behavior.

      My approach to understanding the evolution of brain and behavior was to look at the influences of the basal ganglia and the physiological stress response on the expression of stereotyped social displays in the green anole.  The display repertoire of the green anole is much like other species that involve a conservative “base” display evoked in many contexts that serves as the core element in other displays that are more precisely controlled and convey more specific information.  Such adaptive variations on a theme, to the extent that their substrate is understood, can suggest specific hypotheses about the mutual influences of brain and behavior in evolution.  

      For example, how do the psychoactive properties of the hormones involved in the physiological stress response affect the pathway to the basal ganglia or one of its parallel loops with the thalamus or cortex?   Most displays are“motor programs” and often involve both highly stereotyped as well as more flexible elements, depending on the context and stimulus.  Motor programs range in complexity from strings of reflexes through automatized learned behavior.  An additional influence, that of reproductive status and the presence of relevant hormones, has been relatively neglected but is almost certainly deeply involved. For example, sex steroid hormones, like those of stress, can affect virtually every major component of the path from input to action.    How does control of a specific unit of behavior “shift” from internal control of a fragment of a motor pattern or an autonomic phenomenon to external control by a specific stimulus and or a specific environmental context. More specifically, how do behavioral responses to indistinct but arousing stimuli become progressively more specific in their control?  Here is where an understanding of the stress response may be of value, since responses to potentially challenging perturbations are often hierarchically arranged.  In such a scheme, a minor disturbance evokes a modest response, and progressively more challenging disturbances evoke responses at progressively higher levels of organization.

 

6.1  Fixity and flexibility: How are the functions of units of behavior transformed? 

      A perspective that can illuminate some of the most compelling questions –those related to how units of behavior come to have their communicative function– was engaged by the ethologist Desmond Morris [Morris 1956] almost 50 years ago.   In his review of “ritualization,” the evolutionary changes that result in communicative displays, Morris identifies and describes somatic and autonomic units of behavior.  Somatic units such as fragments of motor programs and autonomic responses such as the green anole’s body color changes, either individually or as a coordinated ensemble, were initially associated with relatively nonspecific phenomena.  Among the autonomic responses,  Morris identified alimentary (changes in salivation, sphincter control, urination, defecation), circulatory (pallor, flushing, vasodilation of organs, fainting),  respiratory (changes in rate or amplitude, sighing, panting. vocalizing), and thermoregulatory (panting, sweating, pilomotor) responses. 

      Morris also iterated the most common kinds of changes that could occur to isolate or emphasize a unit of behavior, including  changes in thresholds,  rhythmic repetition, exaggeration of certain components of the movement, omission of components,  “freezing” of movements,   changes in sequence or in coordination of components, and change in speed or vigor of performance.  The known specific and non-specific effects of stress-related hormones on the nervous system can contribute substantially to hypotheses about how such changes are effected.  For a recently discussed example, an acute stress episode can impair the ability of “higher” neural centers to inhibit “conservative” patterns of behavior controlled by lower centers [see Arnsten 1998].  Basic information about how specific aspects of the stress response affect specific neural areas may provide the key to understanding the control and evolution of core theme, variation, and how modifiers act in display repertoires.  Although it is reasonable that adaptive variations in the regional distribution of neurotransmitter and hormone receptors play a large role in evolutionary change, there is as yet little comparative data.  Although of great intrinsic interest for understanding the neuromodulatory influence of experience on brain function, the basic information being provided for the brain of the green anole by researchers such as Cliff Summers and colleagues [see Summers 2001, Summers 2002] may prove to be of comparably great value for comparative studies and insight into evolutionary processes

6.2.  The stress response

      Broadly construed, stressors are any of a large array of real or perceived challenges to an organisms’s ability to meet its real or perceived needs.  These challenges activate an ensemble of coordinated physiological coping mechanisms collectively called the stress response.   Traditional definitions of stress have historically been rooted in a medical model and typically focus on coping with challenges to homeostasis [for example, Johnson, et al 1992].  While arguably the most compelling of needs, homeostasis is, in terms of an animal’s Darwinian fitness, only the most urgent of several needs.  The broader definition used here avoids the limitations of  traditional  models and more fully accommodates Hans Selye’s original vision [Selye 1980] as well recent views  such as that of McEwen [McEwen 1999] who sees stress as “a threat, real or implied, to the psychological or physiological integrity of an individual.”   Similarly, Mac Hadley [Hadley 1996], in his popular textbook, wrote, “Discrepancies between perceptions of internal or external circumstances and innate or acquired expectations lead to patterned stress responses . . ..”  Such definitions (see also [Goldstein 1990 and Levine 1993]) allow the extension of insights from medically oriented research to the growing interest in subclinical expression of stress and its subtle if relentless influence on the evolution of life histories [see Greenberg 2002b, Greenberg, et al 2002].

      The stress response involves fairly well understood phases that provide both rapid response and long-term accommodation. The rapid system involves an ensemble of responses centered on the sympatho-adrenomedullary system (SAMS), involving release of epinephrine and norepinephrine from specialized extensions of the sympathetic nervous system, adrenal chromaffin tissue (adrenal medulla in mammals).  Continued (or frequently repeated) stressors then activate the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. (Fig 1).  Although the stress response is prominently associated with coping with significant threats to survival, it is important to note that many coping responses are “subclinical” and are manifest mainly in modest, sometimes difficult to detect, adjustments of tone in an endocrine or neurophysiological system.  Further, most hormones are “pleiotropic” in that they have multiple effects some of which may be unrelated to the phenomenon that evoked them (below). 

6.3.  Hormonal pleiotropy

      It is significant that most hormones are pleiotropic –they manifest multiple effects. Hormone release may have been evoked in a specific adaptive context, but their other (“collateral”) effects may or may not complement or support the primary effect.  In any event, they are available to be transformed or incorporated into other adaptive traits, including life history habits or social displays [See Greenberg, et al 2002, and references therein].  This diversity of hormone effects in concert with variations in the distribution of receptors on neurons in different parts of the brains of closely related species [see for example, Shaw and Kennedy 2002] indicates an important emerging perspective on the evolution of species-specific differences.

      Relevant examples of the multiple –pleiotropic– effects of hormones are provided by adrenal corticosterone and the pituitary hormone that causes its release, corticotropin (adreno-corticotrophic hormone, ACTH); they each have independent psychoactive effects that include amelioration of aggressive responses, at least in rodents [Leshner and Politch 1979].  In our lizard, the stress of social subordination may be responsible for reduced androgen [Greenberg and Crews 1990] and reduced motivation to court.  When a dominant male is removed from a laboratory vivarium that he has cohabited with a subordinate, the recovery of interest in courtship may take many hours or even days [Greenberg, et al 1995].  Interestingly, if testosterone in subordinates is artificially increased by means of an implant placed before the dominant-subordinate relationship is established, the subordinate will court as soon as the dominant is removed (unpublished observations) – a situation that may be much more like that in nature, and consistent with observations of the effects of testosterone on arousal and attention [see Andrew 1976].

      Stress results in elevated circulating corticosterone. One of its potential consequences of the hypothalamic corticotropin (ACTH) needed to stimulate release of this adrenal steroid is a collateral release of melanotropin (melanocyte-stimulating hormone, MSH) [Proulx-Ferland, et al 1982] and this is detectable in the blood of subordinate animals [Greenberg and Chen 1987].   Melanotropin has positive effects that aid in growth and recovery from trauma and psychoactive properties that reduce aggression.  In addition, subordinate animals select different perch sites than dominates where the effect of the darkening effect of melanotropin on dermal chromatophores may provide a significant survival advantage [Greenberg 2002b and references therein]. 

      Stressors that challenge homeostasis, the most urgent of needs, are the best known but by no means the only experiences that can activate the stress response. Further, the direct effects of coping mechanisms frequently have collateral effects that may or may not reinforce each other. Indeed, a collateral effect of a specific hormone might well serve other needs.  The evolutionary process is intellectually fascinating in part because of its capacity for making the most of available resources to serve adaptive needs, a process sometimes nicknamed “bricolage,” after the French term (bricoloeur) for a handyman able to make a virtue of necessity.

 

6.4.  Stereotyped behavior, stereotypies, stress, and the basal ganglia.  

      Species-typical displays and clinical stereotypies are related not only by the fixity of expression but by their responsiveness to stress. All contexts in which green anoles display reasonably involve elevated arousal and at least a mild stress response, and the “core” display all others are related to (“assertion”) can be performed even in the absence of specific stimuli, but always in situations of elevated alertness. 

      Dysfunctional compensations such as stereotypies, addictions, neuroses and psychoses are all known to be affected by the stress response. This is reasonable given the well know psychoactive effects of stress-sensitive hormones on alertness and arousal as the organism under stress adjusts to enhance its assessment of potential environmental stressors. The physiological stress response, in its fullest expression can also affect integrative and efferent components of behavior. Altogether, we can expect enhanced  arousal and vigilance, lowered sensory thresholds, increased attention width and capacity for sustained attention, and conservatism in the perceived salience of stimuli.  These are all stress-sensitive aspects of behavior [references in Greenberg, et al 2002, Table 1], so it is unsurprising that energized or aroused lizards may repeat stereotyped patterns frequently. But there is as yet no clarity as to where in the circuit from input to output the stress hormones are active. Some clues are likely to emerge from examinations of regional neurotransmitter changes correlated with behavior [Summers 2001, Summers 2002] and regional changes in metabolism detected by various imaging technologies [Baxter 2001, this issue].

      Clues will also emerge from fuller understanding of the causes of clinical stereotypies in which repetition is clearly inappropriate or dysfunctional.  Most dysfunctional stereotypies are manifest in abnormal contexts such as zoos or laboratories or as a result of stress where they are often viewed as evoked by stress or an errant attempt at stress-reduction [Broverman 1974, Cooper and Nicol 1991, Cooper and Nicol 1993, but see Mason 1991 for a critical review].

      Such dysfunctional stereotypies may be unlike only in degree from the adaptive expressions of stereotyped behavior observed to be spontaneously expressed in natural habitats. The form of such ethological stereotypies, often correspond to the termed “fixed action patterns”(FAP) of early ethologists [Tinbergen 1951] which were presumed to be genetically determined responses to specific stimuli (a “sign stimulus” or “releaser”).  FAPs also resemble clinical stereotypies in that although they may be shaped by external influences (and to that extent “modified by experience” and therefore “learned”) they complete themselves with relative independence of external feedback – They will continue until their pattern is concluded even though their functional ends have been accomplished. 

      Why should we suspect that the performance of a stereotyped display or even a dysfunctional stereotypy be stress reducing?   Real or perceived familiarity and a sense of control are additional variables in the stress response that must color an interpretation of anxiety.  Recalling Seligman’s views of the modulation of the stress response by perceived helplessness [e.g., Seligman 1975a, Seligman 1975b], the apparent “controllability” of a stress-evoking situation is at the heart of Geralt Huether’s [Huether 1996] concept of a “central adaptation syndrome.”  In Huether’s view, different coping strategies are effected depending on the animal’s perception of the controllability of the stressor.  Controllable situations refine existing strategies while uncontrollable situations can cause changes in behavioral responsiveness and a reorganization of neural circuits affecting learning – an “adaptive reorganization of the associative brain.”

      Perceived controllability of a stressor was specifically identified as an influence on the basal ganglia system’s mesoaccumbens dopaminergic system [Cabib and Puglisi-Allegra 1996] a phenomenon that might be linked with emerging understanding of the basal ganglia’s role in expectations [Kawagoe, et al 1998, Schultz, et al 1992].

      The strategies of the “central adaptation syndrome” are likely related to those of passive versus active coping strategies evoked to cope with unescapable versus escapable stressors discussed by Bandler and his colleagues [Bandler, et al 2000].  In their work, alternative autonomic strategies (sympathoexcitatory or sympathoinhibitory) were correlated with activity in discrete columns of the midbrain periaquaductal gray [Bandler, et al 2000].

6.5.  Basal ganglia connection: Clues from dysfunction.  

      Stereotyped displays have been compared to obsessive-compulsive behavior [Baxter 1999], possibly associated with the impairment of one of the several parallel thalamo-cortical loops in which the basal ganglia participate.  The architecture of the motor loop, involving a direct pathway (ultimately facilitory) and an indirect (inhibitory) pathway [see Kaji 2001 for a brief review].  In that respect, it is interesting that OCD, like many other neuropsychiatric disorders, is exacerbated by the stress response.  Alternatively, at least in some cases, trauma to the striatum rather than reconfiguration may be implicated in the pathophysiology of OCD. Striatal neurons might be destroyed by prolonged immunologic stress triggering a cross-reaction between antistreptococcal antibodies and striatal neurons [Dinn, et al 2001].  

      The several social displays of green anoles are stereotyped and more-or-less context-dependent. In their form the recall motor plans, in which specific simple acts are performed in set sequences [Marsden 1982].  Sequential triggering can be visual or proprioceptive feedback, but failing that, internal cues can be generated by the motor system [Brotchie, et al 1991].   Interestingly, in Parkinson’s disease, the most prominent of the degenerative disorders involving basal ganglia and responsible for profound deficits in motor control, the deficits in sequencing attributable to faulty basal ganglia can sometimes be over-ridden by external stimuli that demand heightened arousal in the phenomenon know as paradoxical kinesia, which led Brown and Marsden [Brown and Marsden 1998] to hypothesize that the basal ganglia are integral to nonconscious attention.

      Anne Graybiel’s [Graybiel 1997] work extending our understanding of basal ganglia and its adaptive possibilities has led her to hypothesize that the sequences of units organized by central pattern generators of the motor system are complemented by “cognitive pattern generators” and she suggested that “by analogy with the central pattern generators of the motor system . . . these pattern generators operate to organize neural activity underlying aspects of action-oriented cognition.  Disorders of the basal ganglia may thereby contribute to neural circuit dysfunctions that are expressed as positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia.”  A specific mode of basal ganglia influence is indicated by the observation that an apparent imbalance of activation between the neurochemical zones of the striatum –the striosomes and the matrix in which they are embedded– can result in stereotypies. When applied psychomotor stimulants were applied in concert with dopamine receptor agonists, the degree of motor stereotypy manifest by rats could be predicted by the imbalance created between activity of striosomes and their matrix [Canales and Graybiel 2000].

6.6.  Proximate and Ultimate Causation of Behavior

      It is an ethological truism that questions about “how” behavior is caused and regulated involves proximate factors such as physiological mechanisms while “why” questions emphasize the adaptive and therefore evolutionary significance of behavior, the so called “ultimate” factors.  Between these extremes, developmental and ecological factors abound and must also be considered if behavior is to be most fully understood. Ethology is a profoundly interdisciplinary enterprise.

      The proximate expression of behavior is frequently viewed as the outcome of a hierarchical organization.  The most proximate causes of overt behavior is the activity at a neuromuscular junction. Working backwards, then, from a manifest action we are often led to more central neural structures and pathways.  For example, beginning with the ceratohyoid muscle that controls the dewlap of a related (giant) anole’s dewlap, a path could be traced by a retrograde back to the green anole’s extension of it’s dewlap, a path can be traced to motoneurons in nucleus ambiguus, an element in the brainstem motor system associated with pharyngeal and laryngeal muscles [Font 1991], and vocalization and swallowing in higher vertebrates.

      The specific paths that information takes from the afferent stimulus to the efferent action are all more or less responsive to modulating agents such as the powerfully psychoactive hormones associated with the stress response.  Motor systems are often viewed as hierarchical, such that activation at a relatively centrally located level of limited activation diverges to affect progressively more peripheral levels until they get to final expression.  Unlike a simple “military” hierarchy, however, there is also converging information from other sources of information as well as information flow in the opposite direction to effect a feedback consolidation or reconfiguring of the activity along the path. 

      Although the behavioral system in which causes and consequences are envisioned is immensely rich and there are numerous sites at which alternative actions and interactions can be brought into play, their ultimate causation is constrained by history and thereby limits the possible evolutionary mechanisms we can propose.   The ultimate consequences of this richness, on the other hand, can only be imagined.  The manifest adaptive functions of displays, with their expressions of fixity and flexibility tightly correlated with specific environmental contexts, are among the most likely of social phenomena to yield significant insights into the past processes and future possibilities of the coevolution of brain and behavior.

 

 

 

Acknowledgments.  I am grateful to an anonymous reviewer and three recent meetings that provided opportunities to explore the implications of the work and ideas reviewed here with colleagues in allied disciplines. The catalytic effects of such interdisciplinary meetings cannot be underestimated.  The Across Species Comparisons and Psychopathology group met in Boston, July 16 and 17, 1999 [proceedings published, Cory and Gardner 2002]; The Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology (formerly the American Society of Zoologists) hosted a three day symposium devoted to stress during their annual meeting in Chicago, January 3-7, 2001 (proceedings published, [Carr and Summers 2002], and the satellite meeting devoted to the implications of the work of Paul D. MacLean, at the International Behavioral Neuroscience Society in Capri, Italy, June 19-23, 2002 (this issue of Physiology & Behavior).  And above all, I must acknowledge my gratitude for the empowering and inspirational leadership of Paul D. MacLean.

 

REFERENCES

Andrew, R.J. Attentional processes and animal behaviour, In Growing Points in Ethology. P.P.G. Bateson and R.A. Hinde, Editors. 1976, Cambridge Univ Press, Cambridge. p. 95-133.

Arnsten, A.F.T.  The biology of being frazzled. Science. 280(5370) (1998), pp. 1711-1712.

Bandler, R., K. Keay, N. Floyd, and J. Price, Central circuits mediating patterned autonomic activity during active vs. passive emotional coping. Brain Res Bull. 53 (2000), pp. 95-104.

Baxter, Lewis R., Jr., Brain Mediation of Anolis Social Dominance Displays III. Differential Forebrain 3H-Sumatriptan Binding in Dominant vs. Submissive Males. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 57(4) (2001), pp. 202-213

Baxter, Lewis R., Jr., Serotonin and brain circuitry mediating ritualistic territorial displays in anmiotes, from reptiles to humans. In: T. Insel and M. George, Editors, Soc Biol Psychiatr Ann Meeting, Workshop on studies stemming from the life work of Dr. Paul MacLean. 1999. Washington, D.C.

P. Brotchie, R. Iansek, and M.K. Horne, Motor functions of the monkey globus pallidus. 2. Cognitive aspects of movement and phasic neuronal activity. Brain. 114 (1991), pp. 1685-1702.

D.M. Broverman, E.L. Klaiber, W. Vogal, and Y. Kobayashi, Short-term versus long-term effects of adrenal hormones on behaviors. Psychol. Bull. 81 (1974), pp. 672-694.

P. Brown and C.D. Marsden, What do the basal ganglia do? The Lancet. 351(9118) (1998), pp. 1801-1804.

S. Cabib and S. Puglisi-Allegra, Stress, depression and the mesolimbic dopamine system. Psychopharmacol (Berl). 128(4) (1996), pp. 331-342.

J.J. Canales and A.M. Graybiel, A measure of striatal function predicts motor stereotypy. Nature Neurosci. 3(4) (2000), pp. 377-383.

J.A. Carr and C.H. Summers, Is Stress more than a disease? A comparative look at the adaptiveness of stress. Integr and Comp Biol. 42 (2002), pp. 505-507.

E.C. Clark and J. Baxter, Lewis R., Mammal-Like Striatal Functions in Anolis I. Distribution of Serotonin Receptor Subtypes, and Absence of Striosome and Matrix Organization. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 56(5) (2000a), pp. 235-248.

E.C. Clark, et al., Mammal-Like Striatal Functions in Anolis II. Distribution of Dopamine D1 and D2 Receptors, and a Laminar Pattern of Basal Ganglia Sub-Systems. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 56(5) (2000b), pp. 249-258.

J.J. Cooper and C. Nicol, Stereotypic behaviour affects environmental preference in bank voles, Clethrionomys glareolus, in Anim. Behav. 1991. p. 971-977.

J.J. Cooper and C.J. Nicol, The ‘coping’ hypothesis of stereotypic behaviour:  a reply to Rushen, in Anim. Behav. 1993. p. 616-618.

G.A. Cory Jr. and R. Gardner Jr., The evolutionary neuroethology of Paul MacLean. Praeger, Westport, Conn. (2002).

D. Crews, The Hormonal Control of Behavior in a Lizard. Scientific American. 241 (1979), pp. 180-187.

D. Crews, Interrelationships among ecological, behavioral, and neuroendocrine processes in the reproductive cycle of Anolis carolinensis and other reptiles. Advances in Studies of Behavior. 11 (1980), pp. 1-74.

W.M. Dinn, C.L. Harris, K.M. McGonigal, and R.C. Raynard, Obsessive-compulsive disorder and immunocompetence. Int J Psychiatry Med. 31 (2001), pp. 311-320.

H. Distel, Behavioral responses to the electrical stimulation of the brain in the green iguana, In Behavior and Neurology of Lizards. N. Greenberg and P.D. MacLean, Editors. 1978, Nat. Inst. Ment. Health, Rockville, MD. p. 135-147.

Evans, L.T. A study of a social hierarchy in the lizard Anolis carolinensis. J. Genetic Psychol. 48 (1936), pp. 88-111.

Font, Enrique.  Localization of brainstem motoneurons involved in dewlap extension in the lizard, Anolis equestris. Behav Brain Res. 45(2) (1991), pp. 171-176.

Goldstein, D.S.  1990.  Neurotransmitters and stress. Biofeedback Self Regul  15(3):243_271

Graybiel, A.M. The basal ganglia and cognitive pattern generators. Schizophr Bull. 23(3) (1997), pp. 459-469.

B. Greenberg and G.K. Noble, Social behavior of the American chameleon (Anolis carolinensis Voight). Physiological Zoology. 17(4) (1944), pp. 392-439.

Greenberg, Neil.  A neuroethological investigation of display behavior in the lizard, Anolis carolinensis, (Lacertilia, Iguanidae). American Zoologist. 17(1) (1977), pp. 191-201.

Greenberg, Neil.  An ethogram of the blue spiny lizard, Sceloporus cyanogenys (Reptilia, Lacertilia, Iguanidae). Journal of Herpetology. 11(2) (1977), pp. 177- 195.

Greenberg, Neil.  Ethological considerations in the experimental study of lizard behavior, In Behavior and Neurology of Lizards. N. Greenberg and P.D. MacLean, Editors. 1978, NIMH, Bethesda, MD. p. 203-226.

Greenberg, Neil, A forebrain atlas and stereotaxic technique for the lizard Anolis carolinensis. Journal of Morphology. 174(2) (1982), pp. 217-236.

Greenberg, Neil.  Exploratory behavior and stress in the lizard, Anolis carolinensisZeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 1985; 70:89-102.

Greenberg, Neil.  The saurian psyche revisited: Lizards in research, In The Care and Use of Amphibians, Reptiles, and Fish in Research. K.K. D.O. Schaeffer, L. Krulish, Editor. 1992, Scientists Center for Animal Welfare, Bethesda, MD. p. 75-91.

Greenberg, Neil.  Ethologically informed design in research, In Health and Welfare of Captive Reptiles. C. Warwick, F.L. Frye, and J.B. Murphy, Editors. 1995, Chapman & Hall, London. p. 239-262.

Greenberg, Neil Adaptive Functions of the Corpus Striatum: The Past and Future of the R-Complex in The Neuroethology of Paul MacLean: Frontiers and Convergences. ed. G. Cory and R. Gardner Praeger, London (2002a), 45-81.

Greenberg, Neil Ethological Aspects of Stress in a Model Lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Integrat Compar Biol. 42(3) (2002b), pp. 526-540.

Greenberg, Neil.  et al., Reptile models for biomedical research, In Animal Models in Biomedical Research. A.D. Woodhead, Editor. 1989, CRC Press, New York. p. 289-308.

Greenberg, Neil.  J.A. Carr, and C.H. Summers, Ethological Causes and Consequences of the Stress Response. Integrative and Comparative Biology. 42(3) (2002), pp. 508-516.

Greenberg, Neil and T. Chen, Aggression and social submissiveness alter melanotropin (MSH) in the lizard. Amer Zool. 27(4) (1987), pp. 49A.

Greenberg, Neil.  T. Chen, and D. Crews, Social Status, gonadal state, and the adrenal stress response in the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Horm. behav. 18 (1984), pp. 1-11.

N. Greenberg and D. Crews, Endocrine and behavioral responses to aggression and social dominance in the green anole lizard, Anolis carolinensis. General and Comparative Endocrinology. 77 (1990), pp. 1-10.

Greenberg, Neil.  D. Crews, C. Summers, and J. Harris. Adaptive responses to social subordination. In: XXIVth International Ethology Conference. 1995. Honolulu, Hawaii.

Greenberg, Neil.  P.D. MacLean, and L.F. Ferguson, Role of the Paleostriatum in species-typical display of the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Brain Res Bull. 172 (1979), pp. 229-241.

M.E. Hadley, Endocrinology. 4th edition ed Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ (1996).

M.D. Hauser, The Evolution of Communication. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA (1996).

D.K. Hews and R.A. Worthington, Fighting from the right side of the brain: Left visua;l field preference during aggression in free-ranging male tree lizards (Urosaurus ornatus). Brain, Behavior, and Evolution. 58 (2001), pp. 356-361.

 Heymer, A. Ethological Dictionary. Verlag Paul Parey, Berlin (1977).

G. Huether, The central adaptation syndrome: psychosocial stress as a trigger for adaptive modifications of brain structure and brain function. Progress in Neurobiology. 48(6) (1996), pp. 569-612.

T.A. Jenssen, Display diversity in anoline lizards and problems of interpretation, In Behavior and Neurology of Lizards. N. Greenberg and P.D. MacLean, Editors. 1978, National Institute of Mental Health, Rockville, MD. p. 269-286.

T.A. Jenssen, Display modifiers of Anolis opalinus (Sauria, Iguanidae). Herpetologica. 35 (1979), pp. 21-30.

T.A. Jenssen, N. Greenberg, and K.A. Hovde, Behavioral profile of free-ranging  lizards, Anolis carolinensis, across breeding and post-breeding seasons. Herpetol. Monographs. 9 (1995), pp. 41-62.

T.A. Jenssen, M.B. Lovern, and J.D. Congden, Field testing the protandry-based mating system for the lizard, Anolis carolinensis: does the model organism have the right model? Behav. ecol. Sociobiol. 50 (2001), pp. 162-172.

T.A. Jenssen, K. Orrell, and M.B. Lovern, Sexual dimorphisms in aggressive signal structure and use by a polygynous lizard (Anolis carolinensis). Copeia. 2000 (2000), pp. 140-149.

E.O. Johnson, T.C. Kamilaris, G.P. Chrousos, and P.W. Gold, Mechanisms of stress: A dynamic overview of hormonal and behavioral homeostasis. Neurosci and Biobehav Rev. 16 (1992), pp. 115-130.

R. Kaji, Basal ganglia as a sensory gating devise for motor control. J. med. Investig. 48 (2001), pp. 142-146.

R. Kawagoe, Y. Takakawa, and O. Hikosaka, Expectation of reward modulates cognitive signals in the basal ganglia. Nature Neuoscience. 1(5) (1998), pp. 411-416.

L.H. Kleinholz, Studies in reptilian color change II. The pituitary and adrenal glands in the regulation of the melanophores of Anolis carolinensis. J Exper Zool. 15 (1938a), pp. 474-491.

L.H. Kleinholz, Studies in reptilian color change III. Control of light phase and behavior of isolated skin. Journal Exper. Zool. 15 (1938b), pp. 492-499.

W.J. Korzan, T.R. Summers, and C.H. Summers, Manipulation of visual sympathetic sign stimulus modifies social status and plasma catecholamines. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 128(2) (2002), pp. 153-161.

E.T. Larson and C.H. Summers, Serotonin reverses dominant social status. Behav Brain Res. 121 (2001), pp. 95-102.

A.I. Leshner and J.A. Politch, Hormonal control of submissiveness in mice: Irrelevance of the androgens and relevance of the pituitary-adrenal hormones. Physiol & Behav. 22 (1979), pp. 531-534.

S. Levine, The psychoendocrinology of stress. Annals of the N Y Academy of Science. 697 (1993), pp. 61-69.

P.D. MacLean, Effects of lesions of globus pallidus on species-typical display behavior of squirrel monkeys. Brain Res Bull. 149 (1978), pp. 175-196.

P.D. MacLean, The Triune Brain in Evolution. Plenum, New York (1990).

C.D. Marsden, The mysterious motor function of the basal ganglia. Neurology. 32 (1982), pp. 514-539.

G.J. Mason, Stereotypies:  a critical review. Anim. Behav. 41 (1991), pp. 1015-1037.

J.M. Matter, P.J. Ronan, and C.H. Summers, Central monoamines in free-ranging lizards: Differences associated with social roles and territoriality. Brain behav Evol. 51 (1998), pp. 23-32.

B.S. McEwen, Stress.  In: Wilson, RA, Keil F, general editors.  The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences (MITECS).   1999.

D. Morris, The feather postures of birds and the problem of the origin of social signals, in Behavior. 1956. p. 75-113.

G.K. Noble and H.T. Bradley, The mating behavior of lizards; its bearing on the theory of sexual selection. Ann New York Acad Sci. 35 (1933), pp. 25-100.

A. Parent, Comparative neurobiology of the basal ganglia. Wiley, New York (1986).

Proulx-Ferland L, Labrie F, Dumont D, Cote J, Coy DH, Sveiraf J. 1982.  Corticotropin-releasing factor stimulates secretion of melanocyte-stimulating hormone from the rat pituitary.  Science. 217(4554):62-63.

T.C. Schnierla, The relationship between observation and experimentation in the field study of behavior. Ann New York Acad Sci. 51 (1950), pp. 1022-1044.

W. Schultz, P. Apicella, E. Scarnati, and T. Ljungberg, Neuronal activity in monkey ventral striatum related to the expectation of reward. J Neurosci. 12(12) (1992), pp. 4595-4610.

M. Seligman, Helplessness. Freeman & Co., San Francisco (1975a).

M. Seligman, R. Rosellini, and M. Kozak, Learned helplessness in the rat. J. compar. physiol. Psychology. 88 (1975b), pp. 542-547.

H. Selye, The stress concept today, In Handbook on Stress and Anxiety. I.L. Kutash and L.B.a.a. Schlesinger, Editors. 1980, Jossey-Bass publishers, San Francisco. p. 127-143.

B.K. Shaw and G.G. Kennedy, Evidence for species differences in the pattern of androgen receptor distribution in relation to species differences in an androgen-dependent behavior. J. Neurobiol. 52 (2002), pp. 203-220.

R.A. Sugerman and L.S. Demski, Agonistic behavior elicited by electrical stimulation of the brain in western collared lizards, Crotaphytus collaris. Brain Behav Evol. 15 (1978), pp. 446-469.

C.H. Summers, Mechanisms for quick and variable responses. Brain Behav Evol. 57 (2001), pp. 283-292.

C.H. Summers, Social interaction over time, implications for stress responsiveness. Int Comp Biol. 42 (2002), pp. 591-599.

C.H. Summers and N. Greenberg, Somatic correlates of adrenergic activity during aggression in the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Horm behav. 28(1) (1994), pp. 29-40.

C.H. Summers and N. Greenberg, Activation of central biogenic amines following aggressive interactions in male lizards, Anolis carolinensis. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 45 (1995), pp. 339-349.

C.H. Summers, et al., Regional and temporal separation of serotonergic activity mediating social stress. Neuroscience. 87(2) (1998), pp. 489-496.

R.S. Tarr, Species typical display behavior following stimulation of the reptilian striatum. Physiol Behav. 29 (1982), pp. 615-620.

N. Tinbergen, The Study of Instinct. Clarendon Press, Oxford (1951).

S. Winberg, G.E. Nilsson, and K.H. Olsen, Changes in brain serotonergic activity during hierarchic behavior in Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus L.) are socially induced. J Comp Physiol A. 170 (1992), pp. 93-99.

U. Yodyingyuad, C. de la Riva, J.H. Abbott, and E.B. Keverne, Relationship between dominance hierarchy, cerebrospinal fluid levels of amine transmitter metabolites (5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid and homovanillic acid) and plasma cortisol in monkeys. Neuroscience. 16 (1985), pp. 851-858.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

TABLE 1.

Inventory of social behavior in Anolis carolinensis

click here for “open” ethogram

(the “open ethogram” will be updated as related observations and insights of collegaues engaged in Anolis behavioral research become available; the “original” ethogram may be viewed in the PDF version of the original paper)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

TABLE 2

Shared Elements of Social Displays in the Green Anole

                                             Display Components                                                                          

Display Context

Head-nod

push-up

Dewlap

Extended Throat

Sagittal Expansion

Rapid  Nodding

“Subordination”

T

 

 

 

 

 

“Assertion”

T

T

T

 

 

 

“Threat”

T

T

T

T

 

 

“Challenge”

T

T

T

T

T

 

“Courtship”

T

T

T

 

 

T

                                                                                   

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

FIGURE 1.  The effects of stress on a dermal chromatophore of Anolis carolinensis. Acute and chronic stressors are integrated to cause the release of hormones that interact with each other and converge in affecting the darkness of a chromatophore. ACTH, adrenocorticotropic hormone; CS, corticosterone; E, epinephrine; MSH, melanocyte stimulating hormone; NE, norepinephrine. CS can elevate the ratio of E to NE by facilitating a key enzyme within the adrenal gland. E stimulates receptors preferentially and then receptors resulting in opposing effects. (adapted from Greenberg 2002b)