A&O – COGNITION – STATES of MIND and THEORY of MIND

 

ART & ORGANISM

 

STATES of MIND:

What are Other People THINKING?

or FEELING?

The Development and Deployment of Theory of Mind

 (and its connections to INTERSUBJECTIVITY)

“MEANING” of anything arguably derives from its CONNECTIONS: in conjunctions with STATES of MIND, read A&O notes on CONNECTIONS BETWEEN INDIVIDUALS

 

THINKING and FEELING are what people (among other organisms) do.  It is how they experience the universe outside of personal consciousness.

.

Difficult point: the boundaries between our self and the world seems simple: “our skin.”  But the reciprocal interactions between organisms and the environments they inhabit make those boundaries on occasion seem very indistinct.  (Jill Taylor mentions this in her “Stroke of Insight” video).  It is not so rare an experience for individuals to have a vivid sense of “being one with everything.”  On other occasions, we sense our intersubjectivity … connections with another person–“being of the same mind” when we think alike or “two hearts that beat as one” when we seem to have the same feelings.

 Many of us pursue that feeling.  BUT WE as organisms consider that individual boundaries enable constructive or critical thinking about how to be–how to behave. So we intuitively set aside those connections (phenomenologists call this “bracketing“).  The diversity of things contained within our skin communicate with with each other more easily than they communicate with what is outside.  Connections are within and between “levels of organization” and the individual person is one such level. 

.

What can Artifacts Tell Us — or “WHAT WERE THEY THINKING?” (and how would we know). Insight into the evolution of our own competencies is what we seek here. What can art reveal about the artist’s STATE of MIND?  (see A&O page on ART & ARTIFACT)

While typically, artists seek transcendence in their work, all acts have the potential to provide an artifact of great significance, depending on the disposition and experience of the recipient. Such artifacts can be corporeal or in memory only. 

The main emphasis of DEEP Ethology is behavior but we can look at artifacts and do our best to infer the behavioral patterns and state of mind that led to them.  We must exercise our  capacity to express a “theory of mind.”  “…the ability to attribute mental states–beliefs, intents, desires, emotions, knowledge, etc.—to oneself, and to others, and to understand that others have beliefs, desires, intentions, and perspectives that are different from one’s own.[1] Theory of mind is crucial for everyday social interactions and is used when analyzing, judging, and inferring others’ behaviors.[2]  (this is the aspect emphasized by Thomas Hobbes (1651) about the function of consciousness.)

Deficits can occur in people with autism spectrum  disorders, schizophreniaattention deficit hyperactivity disorder,[3] cocaine addiction,[4] and brain damage suffered from alcohol’s neurotoxicity.[5] (Wikipedia)

CULTURAL RELATIVITY?  “While this sequence represents the general trend in skill acquisition, it seems that more emphasis is placed on some skills in certain cultures, leading to more valued skills to develop before those that are considered not as important. For example, in individualistic cultures such as the United States, a greater emphasis is placed on the ability to recognize that others have different opinions and beliefs. In a collectivistic culture, such as China, this skill may not be as important and therefore may not develop until later.”

 


      WHAT ARE OTHER PEOPLE FEELING?

  •  
  • INTERSUBJECTIVITY (an aspect of INDIVIDUATION X SOCIALIZATION) — so we know a bit about mirror neurons; theory of mind; empathy– NOW, maybe you really can feel someone else’s pain: Researchers have found that the rat brain activates the same cells when they observe the pain of others as when they experience pain themselves. In addition, without activity of these ‘mirror neurons,’ the animals no longer share the pain of others. Finding the neural basis for sharing the emotions of others is an exciting step towards understanding empathy.”  (popular link) (technical link)

Arguably, a need for intersubjective meaning is close to the essence of what it means to be human;  we intuit this loss at a deep existential level when we posit a reason for someone’s distress or even dysfunctional behavior as “because they are lonely.”  Here we recall Aristotle’s’ observation: He who is unable to live in society, or who has no need because he is sufficient for himself, must be either a beast or a god.  (Aristotle 384-322 BC, Politics bk. 1, 1253a 27-9) [i]  Look more deeply into this most poignant of needs: start at A&O post on LonelinessWikipedia on Loneliness, then go back to AO-needs-sociality-and-physiology-loneliness/  

 


[i]. Mary Shelley’s Frankenstein’s monster (rather than Boris Karloff’s) became a beast because he was deprived of sociality: “My heart was fashioned to be susceptible of love and sympathy—and, when wrenched by misery to vice and hatred, it did not endure the violence of the change without torture, such as you cannot even imagine. . . . I was nourished with high thoughts of honor and devotion.  But now vice has degraded me beneath the meanest animal. . . . “ And his vice?  His appearance invited fear and unjust rejection, shunned and unbearably lonely, the nineteen year old Shelley’s monster was born. (See Steve Gould’s “The Monster’s Human Nature” in Dinosaur in a Haystack).

 


ENCOURAGED by the ARTS?   BUT development never ends and an interesting developmental aspect of theory of mind studied in adults is how it was enriched in adults as a result of their reading literary fiction: 

 

        A Good Read (from excerpts at A&O notes on Connections between Individuals):

      Theory of Mind is the human capacity to comprehend that other people hold beliefs and desires and that these may differ from one’s own beliefs and desires. The currently predominant view is that literary fiction—often described as narratives that focus on in-depth portrayals of subjects’ inner feelings and thoughts—can be linked to theory of mind processes, especially those that are involved in the understanding or simulation of the affective characteristics of the subjects. Kidd and Castano (2013) provide experimental evidence that reading passages of literary fiction, in comparison to nonfiction or popular fiction, does indeed enhance the reader’s performance on theory of mind tasks.

      Abstract.  Understanding others’ mental states is a crucial skill that enables the complex social relationships that characterize human societies. Yet little research has investigated what fosters this skill, which is known as Theory of Mind (ToM), in adults. We present five experiments showing that reading literary fiction led to better performance on tests of affective ToM  and cognitive ToM  compared with reading nonfiction, popular fiction, or nothing at all. Specifically, these results show that reading literary fiction temporarily enhances ToM. More broadly, they suggest that ToM may be influenced by engagement with works of art.”  (From David Comer Kidd and Emanuele Castano ‘s (2013) article “Reading Literary Fiction Improves Theory of  Mind.”)

DEEP ETHOLOGY can help:

  • DEVELOPMENT. 

    Developing infants reach a stage in which they manifest some insight into what another individual is likely thinking.  This is their THEORY-OF-MIND: at an early age (about 3-4 years old) we tend to assume that others have minds much like ours (overview of typical developmental path at a Hanen.org site)[i]

    • A fully developed theory of mind is preceded and incorporates an understanding of attention (at about 8 months; Simon Baron-Cohen).  In fact, sharing selective attention may be, Baron-Cohen’s view, an early motive for all communicating.  ToM  also involves a capacity to infer the intentions of others (the “intentional stance” as defined by Daniel Dennett).  The capacity to imitate may lie “at the origins of both theory of mind and other social-cognitive achievements like perspective-taking and empathy.

      “While much research has been done on infants, theory of mind develops continuously throughout childhood and into late adolescence as the synapses (neuronal connections) in the prefrontal cortex develop. The prefrontal cortex is responsible for planning and decision-making.[26] Children seem to develop theory of mind skills sequentially. The first skill to develop is the ability to recognize that others have diverse desires. Children are able to recognize that others have diverse beliefs soon after. The next skill to develop is recognizing that others have access to different knowledge bases. Finally, children are able to understand that others may have false beliefs and that others are capable of hiding emotions.

  • ECOLOGY.  Consider the context in which the nervous system and its functions develop. 
    • While the sequence of morphological and cognitive changes enables the general trend in skill acquisition, it seems that more emphasis is placed on some skills in certain cultures, leading to more valued skills to develop before those that are considered not as important. For example, in individualistic cultures such as the United States, a greater emphasis is placed on the ability to recognize that others have different opinions and beliefs. In a collectivistic culture, such as China, this skill may not be as important and therefore may not develop until later.”  (Wikipedia)

       

  • EVOLUTION.  Consider the adaptive advantage of knowing what others are thinking .  An early “theory of consciousness
    • (“Whoever looketh into himself and considers what he does when he does think, opine, reason, hope, fear, etc., and upon what grounds, he shall thereby read and know what are the thoughts and passions of all other men upon the like occasions.”—Thomas Hobbes 1651 Leviathan)

 

  • PHYSIOLOGY.  The development of Theory-of-Mind is, as in all cognitive competencies, tied to maturation of brain structures (but everything is maturing more-or-less rapidly (depending on stage of maturation).
    • Efforts to localize networks or nodes of particular significance have identified “… the human temporo-parietal junction [as] involved specifically in reasoning about the contents of another person’s mind” (Saxe & Kanwisher 2003)
    • In light of the principles of embodied cognition (see below) we can expect changes in anatomy and physiology apart from the nervous system itself to have an effect on the extent to which we believe we know what other people are likely thinking.]  We might, in fact, know more explicitly what others are thinking by examining our own body’s response to observing them?  This would be predicted by the phenomenologically oriented idea of EMBODIED COGNITION.]

  

 

[i] How Theory of Mind Develops in Typical Children

During infancy and early childhood, children learn the early skills that they’ll need to develop their theory of mind later on. These skills include the ability to [2,3]:

  • pay attention to people and copy them
  • recognize others’ emotions and use words to express them (“happy”, “sad”, “mad”)
  • know that they are different from other people and have different likes/dislikes from others
  • know that people act according to the things they want
  • understand the causes and consequences of emotions (If I throw my toy, Mom will be mad)
  • pretend to be someone else (like a doctor or a cashier) when they play

Between ages 4-5, children really start to think about others’ thoughts and feelings, and this is when true theory of mind emerges. Children develop theory of mind skills in the following order [1, 4, 5]:

  • Understanding “wanting” – Different people want different things, and to get what they want, people act in different ways.
  • Understanding “thinking” – Different people have different, but potentially true, beliefs about the same thing. People’s actions are based on what they think is going to happen.
  • Understanding that “seeing leads to knowing” – If you haven’t seen something, you don’t necessarily know about it (like the Dad in the example above on the telephone). If someone hasn’t seen something, they will need extra information to understand.
  • Understanding “false beliefs” – Sometimes people believe things that are not true, and they act according to their beliefs, not according to what is really true.
  • Understanding “hidden feelings” – People can feel a different emotion from the one they display.

Children’s theory of mind continues to develop after age five. For the next several years they learn to predict what one person thinks or feels about what another person is thinking or feeling [4]. They also begin to understand complex language that relies on theory of mind, such as lies, sarcasm, and figurative language (like “it’s raining cats and dogs”) [4]. Some experts argue that theory of mind development continues over a lifetime as one has more opportunities to experience people and their behaviour [6, 3].

 


What is the relation of “Theory of Mind” to Psychopathy?

 


[i] How Theory of Mind Develops in Typical Children

During infancy and early childhood, children learn the early skills that they’ll need to develop their theory of mind later on. These skills include the ability to [2,3]:

  • pay attention to people and copy them
  • recognize others’ emotions and use words to express them (“happy”, “sad”, “mad”)
  • know that they are different from other people and have different likes/dislikes from others
  • know that people act according to the things they want
  • understand the causes and consequences of emotions (If I throw my toy, Mom will be mad)
  • pretend to be someone else (like a doctor or a cashier) when they play

Between ages 4-5, children really start to think about others’ thoughts and feelings, and this is when true theory of mind emerges. Children develop theory of mind skills in the following order [1, 4, 5]:

  • Understanding “wanting” – Different people want different things, and to get what they want, people act in different ways.
  • Understanding “thinking” – Different people have different, but potentially true, beliefs about the same thing. People’s actions are based on what they think is going to happen.
  • Understanding that “seeing leads to knowing” – If you haven’t seen something, you don’t necessarily know about it (like the Dad in the example above on the telephone). If someone hasn’t seen something, they will need extra information to understand.
  • Understanding “false beliefs” – Sometimes people believe things that are not true, and they act according to their beliefs, not according to what is really true.
  • Understanding “hidden feelings” – People can feel a different emotion from the one they display.

Children’s theory of mind continues to develop after age five. For the next several years they learn to predict what one person thinks or feels about what another person is thinking or feeling [4]. They also begin to understand complex language that relies on theory of mind, such as lies, sarcasm, and figurative language (like “it’s raining cats and dogs”) [4]. Some experts argue that theory of mind development continues over a lifetime as one has more opportunities to experience people and their behaviour [6, 3].

 


  NEXUS Connecting to